Thursday, 29 July 2010

What do we do about placebo?

ResearchBlogging.org
Body in Mind recently featured a piece on the ‘Moral Dilemma of Offering a Known Placebo’ in which Neil O’Connell talks about how the ‘placebo effect … in part rests on the effects of expectation, belief in the treatment and possibly a re-evaluation by the patient of their symptoms’. He was referring to treatments like acupuncture, electrotherapy and so on, and calls them ‘magic kisses’ because they work in a similar fashion to the ‘Mummy will kiss it better’ treatment I’ve given to my kids when they were younger.  The dilemma lies in the fact that placebo is simply an inert, inactive ‘intervention’ given as if it was active - inevitably requiring deception on the part of the practitioner, and what this can do to things like trust and informed choice by the patient.

So much of what we do as clinicians, particularly physical and occupational therapies, has a limited evidence base.  At the same time, some of the ‘active ingredients’ that have been identified in ‘placebo’ are the very things we are taught to develop – like active listening, instilling positive expectations, helping people re-evaluate their situation.  It’s incredibly difficult to disentangle the ‘active’ from the inactive components of the treatment.

Like Neil, I have concerns about encouraging, even inadvertently, any belief in a mystical, magical ingredient – chi anyone?  I also have concerns about any intervention that suggests the need for an ongoing relationship with a clinician – six-weekly ‘adjustments’ sir?  Or interventions that leave the power (or locus of control to be pedantic) with a gadget or device or substance that someone else needs to operate – three monthly infusions madam?

Dan Moerman’s view of health interventions suggests that every treatment inevitably contains culturally-based elements.  These are the result of an interaction between the person seeking treatment, the social environment in which they live, the treatment setting, the ritual associated with the treatment process, and the interpersonal relationship with the practitioner – everything we do in any healthcare encounter will influence the ‘healing’ or ‘meaning response’ of the patient.

Along with the placebo effect (meaning response), we sometimes forget the nocebo effect – the ill effects that people develop as a result of receiving an inactive treatment.  Take a look at any of the randomised, double-blinded, placebo controlled studies, and in a good one, you’ll see listed all the side effects that people developed when receiving the active treatment – and if you look carefully, you’ll also see the side effects that people developed when receiving the inactive treatment.  It’s entirely likely that along with our very effective, evidence-based treatments, some people will also either fail to respond, or will develop side effects that negate the positive effects of the intervention.

So what on earth do we do about this placebo thing?

Putting my ‘patient’ hat on for a moment, and remember that we are ALL patients at some point in our lives, I know that I want honesty from the practitioner I’m seeing.  I want to choose whether I have a certain treatment – or not.  And I want to know my options.  I’d like to be told about both the side effects and the hopefully positive effects of the treatment.  I want to know the evidence-base for the treatments I get (and if I don’t get told, you can bet, like many of our patients, I’ll be onto the internet and into the journals quick as a flash to find out!)

I don’t want to have a long-term relationship with a practitioner who will want to see me every six weeks or three months, and I don’t want chi (or woo).  I’m not into magic, superstition or intuition.

I’m inevitably going bring all my socially-shaped judgements and beliefs and prejudices into the treatment setting, and I know this is going to influence the outcome.

I’m likely to decide on a particular practitioner on the basis of word of mouth (reputation), what I’ve read from the literature (call that advertising if you will), and I’ll probably decide to return (or not) depending on his or her interpersonal skills – and because I too am influenced by the superficial – I’ll probably be influenced by the decor in his or her rooms and the cost of the treatment!

You see, we’re all influenced by these meaning responses.

So… what to do about placebo?  I, like Neil, hope that as the evidence accumulates, I will throw out the treatments that don’t have solid support from well-constructed RCT’s.  I will be mindful of my reputation and hope to have one that means I’m recognised by my adherence to an evidence-based approach to pain management.  I also hope I’ll always focus on helping people to help themselves, so I don’t inadvertently foster dependence on me.  I won’t be incorporating woo, chi or crystals (at least, not on the basis of current evidence!)  I won’t be using gadgets except as part of helping someone develop their own skills.  If I ask someone to come back after a bit of a break, it will be only to review how they’re going with their own goals, and to help them re-jig their plan for the future.

And I will try to recognise that some people will come to see me and will not ‘get better’ – not because of my approach, but because they have come into treatment with their own beliefs and expectations, their own ‘meaning response’ might interfere with what I’m doing.  Above all, I hope I’ll be honest about what I’m doing and be prepared to change my approach on the basis of science.

That darned placebo – whatever do we do about it? Learn more I hope!

Moerman DE (2002). The meaning response and the ethics of avoiding placebos. Evaluation & the health professions, 25 (4), 399-409 PMID: 12449083
Moerman, D., (2003). Doctors and patients: The role of clinicians in the placebo effect. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine. Vol.19(1), pp. 14-22.
Moerman, D., (2003). “Placebo” versus “meaning”: The case for a change in our use of language. Prevention & Treatment. Vol.6(1), pp. No Pagination Specified

Comments
9 Responses to “What do we do about placebo?”
  1. MM says:

    It appears your approach to providing a placebo treatment is to place yourself in the shoes of the patient, and then provide service in conformity with your own expectations. And you appear to be typical of doctors: trained to make decisions as scientists, disdainful of superstition, and desirous to personally weigh the possible consequences of various courses of action.

    While you and I may not like woo, many patients clearly do. Some people look to doctors or other experts not for information and choices, but answers.

    Why not provide treatment based on the patient’s preferences, not your own? You could provide each patient, prior to being diagnosed or treated, with information regarding the placebo effect and ask whether they would like placebo-like treatments to be incorporated where appropriate.

    Recognizing the limits of medical understanding the and the real effects of placebo treatments doesn’t mean you need to take advantage of patients by seeing them three times a week. As noted in the Mind and Body article, you can suggest relatively inexpensive acts that are otherwise beneficial to a patient–taking a vitamin, eating green leafy vegetables, walking 30 min. a day–even if there is no evidence these acts will help the specific problem at issue.

    Give patients information and let them decide–even on the issue of whether they should be given further information. The alternative may be that patients without adequate information will simply seek out expensive placebo treatments on their own.

      Reply
  2. Ronny says:

    Placebo/nocebo is of no serious clinical value or significance, and lying to patients is never acceptable (perhaps with a tiny handful of short term exceptions, like when dealing with a parent who is strongly suspected of abusing their child, or patients who are tipping over into psychosis and need hospitalising).

    (Of course, this effect should always be controlled for in formal studies. But beyond that, meh.)

      Reply
  3. Bernadette Murray says:

    I chuckled when you admitted that the decor might play a role in your perception of a practitioner.
    Seriously though, I really started thinking about the ethics of the nocebo effect particularly when I caught
    myself mentally entertaining the thought of forwarding an email to a friend with graphic pictures of a particularly
    ghastly outcome from a repetitive behavior. I did not take this action because of course this is manipulation –trying to influence my friend to stop a behavior that I have ever so helpfully identified as being inimical to her health and well-being. And that frankly –TRUTH now–I find irritating and annoying.

    The stupidest use of nocebo IMHO are those chain-mail emails that have magical benefits if only one forwards to the certain stipulated number of recipients; otherwise if one breaks the chain, whamm-o the curse is upon thee.

    Slightly more subtle, in the past I have had a doctor use the “if you do not take this medicine as prescribed, you
    could die” exhortation. I would have preferred a candid tempered discussion of limits of knowledge about the consequences of not treating the condition. Since I did not die when I stopped the taking medication and my test results came back within normal ranges, I unfortunately came to harbor some cognitive beliefs about doctors in my early twenties such as “doctors lie and exaggerate to ensure patient compliance ” and “doctors always minimize how painful a procedure really is so you will agree that the benefit surely outweighs such modest discomfort.” ha!

    I did not think doctors were EVIL or malicious. Seemed to me that they were rationalizing that the end justifies the mean (and they did not believe that they could present evidence to me and expect me to make a rational decision ) so they needed to employ some extreme emotional motivation.

    Fortunately, I have had many more encounters and opportunities to interact with doctors and I realize that those human beings, many of whom have discarded the white coat and are willing to share evidence and discuss options, are not homogeneous in their orientations.

    I have also come to realize that many alternative remedies are marketed in a weird kind of nocebo-fashion as non-existing illnesses are invoked so that the “remedies” can alleviate or cure them. ” A Toxic system filled with free radicals is the underlying cause of all illness….sip on acai juice while experiencing the health benefits of chelation therapy combined with a colonic.” Yup time to exorcize that demon “toxicity”

    Coming back to what you are saying so well in your post: the problem with placebos is the Dumbo’s magic feather conundrum. Dumbo can fly without the feather, and we as patients actually generate our own neurotransmitters which modulate our own systems. I really like your emphasis on interacting with your patients so that they can fly on their own (metaphorically speaking of course ;-)

      Reply
  4. Neil O'Connell says:

    Great post Bronnie (thanks for the mention) and I couldn’t agree more. I think patient choice is a difficult concept here because it is very difficult for patients to access or distinguish good health information. There is so much misdirection, anecdote and outright fraud out there and when you are in desperate need you become vulnerable to the peddlers of false hope. I guess it boils down to “keep what works, keep the care and dump the magic”.

      Reply
  5. ian stevens says:

    Its a difficult issue, ethically prqctically and economically (for both private providers and patients) in many ways. However, positively if we can enhance meaning ,facilitate a persons own process of restitution and understand what may move a person towards this state than this has to be a good thing? Spaces and places where treament takes place for example have been studied by the Rheumatologist Esther Sternberg. Often complex cases of distress and disabilty are shunted into stark clnical rooms with rushed appointments (this was my experience of several pain clinics).
    I fully endorse pain managment , enhancing self efficacy and minimising disability. However in Neils terms a few ‘kisses’ probably will not go amiss. A good narrative which looks at a patients journey dealing occupational therapy following an elbow fracture and secondary neurogenic pain is the Sociologist Ann Oakley’s Fracture. The response Ann obtained through the enhanced treatment effect cultivated by the acupuncturist (environment ,demenour , care ) was in stark contrast to the orthopaedic clinic . People often seek care, reasurance and an ‘individualised’ approach –this is often lacking in many protocol led clinics.
    I disagree with the post that suggest placebo and nocebo are of no clinical importance. For many people nocebo is a feature of many health care interactions and according to Benedetti may have strong negative physiological consequencies. As biomechancial explanations of pain are dominant many people are told or interpret the results of tests,scans etc in often maladpative ways ….you have the spine of a 70 year old often leads to catastrophisation and disability for example.
    I think for some people ‘chi’ used as a metaphorical explanation is not too bad… metaphors used wisely may indeed enchance meaning and downregulate threat. I suggest tai chi and use the principles all the time–it does not mean that I think chi,prana etc are ‘real’ but perhaps these metaphors may improve a persons self image /intereoception (especially if they learn to do things themself) .The danger perhaps is if these things are relied upon too much …..’Peddlars of false hope’ …yes a big problem and it is easy for those in the scientific medicla community to sit on a pedastal casting stones on CAM etc when many interventions offered by the medical profession offer little and in many cases are iatrogenic. I would have no problem suggesting certain ‘woo’ therapies offered by some caring people I know over and above an interventional pain clinc or rheumatology clinic for example . Allowing a stressed out person with often complex social issues a little respite seems fine in some circumstances . It may be a better use of resources than the mri/ct multiple health professional pharmacology route which tends to be a frequent occurrence.
    Sometimes I just think we live in a overly medicalised culture and placebo treatments will just evolve to meet the demands of the population seeking care and attention!

Yep!

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio

Brainstem, spinal cord images hidden in Michelangelo’s Sistine Chapel fresco

Acupuncture Pseudoscience in the NEJM

Here is the conclusion quoted from a recent New England Journal of Medicine (NEJM) review article on acupuncture for back pain:

As noted above, the most recent wellpowered clinical trials of acupuncture for chronic low back pain showed that sham acupuncture was as effective as real acupuncture. The simplest explanation of such findings is that the specific therapeutic effects of acupuncture, if present, are small, whereas its clinically relevant benefits are mostly attributable to contextual and psychosocial factors, such as patients’ beliefs and expectations, attention from the acupuncturist, and highly focused, spatially directed attention on the part of the patient.

Translation – acupuncture does not work. Why, then, are the same authors in the same paper recommending that acupuncture be used for chronic low back pain? This is the insanity of the bizarro world of CAM (complementary and alternative medicine).

Let’s break down their conclusions a bit. They have reviewed the clinical evidence, as I and others have done before, and found that when real acupuncture is compared to various forms of sham acupuncture (the acupuncture version of a placebo) there is no difference. As I have written many times before – it doesn’t matter where you stick the needles, or even if you stick the needles. Since acupuncture consists of sticking needles in acupuncture points, the only reasonable conclusion from this evidence is that there is no specific effect from acupuncture – acupuncture does not work.

The phrase, “contextual and psychosocial factors, such as patients’ beliefs and expectations, attention from the acupuncturist, and highly focused, spatially directed attention on the part of the patient.” is a fancy way of saying “placebo effects.” In other words, there are some non-specific subjective benefits to getting attention from a practitioner. There is this assumption, however, that these benefits are real and worthwhile. However, they are likely to be illusory – an artifact of observation and reporting, not a real improvement in the patient’s condition. In real science-based medicine, that is the underlying assumption – placebo effects are largely illusory – a variable to be controlled for.

But there has been recent controversy over the role of the placebo effect in ethical and evidence-based practice. This is, in my opinion, largely a back door attempt to justify CAM treatments that do not work. The claim is that placebo effects are real and useful. But a systematic review of the placebo effect in clinical trials concluded:

We did not find that placebo interventions have important clinical effects in general. However, in certain settings placebo interventions can influence patient-reported outcomes, especially pain and nausea, though it is difficult to distinguish patient-reported effects of placebo from biased reporting. The effect on pain varied, even among trials with low risk of bias, from negligible to clinically important. Variations in the effect of placebo were partly explained by variations in how trials were conducted and how patients were informed.

In other words – for any objective outcome, there is no important placebo effect. For outcomes that are subjectively reported by patients, there is a highly variable placebo effect. It is plausible that the expectation of benefit could result in the release of dopamine and endorphins and produce a physiological decrease in pain, for example, in a subset of people, and there is some evidence for this. But this is, at best, a transient symptomatic effect – not therapeutic.

Such effects are also non-specific – meaning they do not derive from the intervention itself, but from the therapeutic ritual surrounding the intervention. Even treatments that do not work may therefore provide these non-specific benefits. My opinion is that the non-specific benefits of the ritual of treatment should be combined with an actually effective treatment, not magic pretending to be medicine. There are many reasons for this. One is the ethics of patient autonomy and informed consent – giving a fake treatment to a patient violates the patient’s rights, in my opinion.

Further, there is potential downstream harm from convincing patients that fake magical treatments are effective, because of placebo effects. Then using obscure language to hide the fact that the treatment actually does not work. This distorts the public’s view of medicine, and of what works, and sets them up to be victims of fake treatments when their ailment is not subjective or self-limiting. In other words – refer them to an acupuncturists when they have back pain and they may rely upon acupuncture, or some other non-scientific intervention, when they have a more serious illness.

The authors of this article recommend:

He has specifically requested a referral for acupuncture, and we would suggest a course of 10 to 12 treatments over a period of 8 weeks from a licensed acupuncturist or a physician trained in medical acupuncture.

This contradicts their own conclusions. Why is training in acupuncture necessary? That training largely consists of identifying acupuncture points, knowing which points to use on an individual patient, and knowing the technique of needle insertion – but none of these things matter. The sham ritual is all that matters – you can literally fake it and get the same response. I bet a 10 minute video is all that is necessary. In fact I bet even that is not necessary – you could probably fake it well enough to get a maximum placebo effect without any prior demonstration.

What the authors of this article have done is something that is increasingly common in CAM (when it is trying to infiltrate academia and peer-reviewed journals like the NEJM) – reviewing the evidence, admitting that the CAM treatment does not work, then making an elaborate and misleading appeal to placebo effects, and ending with a recommendation to use the treatment that does not work. Specifically, they not only recommend using the treatment, but in its fullest magical form, complete with all the disproven claims (that is what “medical acupuncture” is). It’s a bait and switch con game, nothing more. Come for the placebo effect, then be treated with magical nonsense.

[Slashdot]

[Digg]

[Reddit]

[del.icio.us]

[Facebook]

[Technorati]

[Google]

[StumbleUpon]

Acupuncture does have beneficial effects with some people. I have recently stuck some needles randomly about my knee that has been hurting intermittently for a year or so and it felt great afterwards (a case study of one!). We know that something happens but no-one knows exactly what. Stick needles anywhere and there's an effect. Well, the brain is probably going to do something in response to this tissue 'invasion' and maybe in certain cases it prioritises away from the pain hence the relief. Some talk about endogenous opioids, adenosine and DNIC. Perhaps they all have a role. Certainly expectation, belief and understanding will have a part to play as in any treatment. Modulation of pain is complex and the notion of local effects is out-dated. Multidimensional responses are more likely (Thacker & Gifford, 2002).

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio

Friday, 23 July 2010

Champagne fizzles out if served with a splash

Champagne being poured into a glassNot the best way to pour champagne.Carlos Alvarez / iStockphoto

If you want to enjoy champagne to the full, pour it out as you would a beer.

Sommeliers and connoisseurs may find the suggestion hard to swallow, but the evidence published in the Journal of Agricultural and Food Chemistry1 seems irrefutable. Pouring champagne into a tilted glass helps it to retain the dissolved gas that is vital for the gustatory experience, say Gérard Liger-Belair and his colleagues at the University of Reims in France.

The French team compared the amount of dissolved carbon dioxide in a fresh flute of champagne poured the traditional way — splashing it into a vertical glass — with that after pouring along the inside of a tilted glass, as one does with beer to avoid giving it too much frothy 'head'. The researchers found that champagne served chilled (at 4 °C) contains about twice as much dissolved CO2 using the beer-like method.

A typical 75-cubic-centimetre bottle of champagne contains five litres of dissolved CO2. When uncorked, the release of pressure means that the liquid becomes supersaturated with the gas, which then begins to escape as bubbles. These contribute to the pleasure of drinking champagne in several ways: they give it a lively appearance, release aromas (the 'nose'), produce the stimulating oral sensation of collapsing bubbles, and create sharp tang owing to the conversion of CO2 to carbonic acid inside the mouth.

"CO2 has a strong effect on the sensory experience and flavour of champagne," says Susan Ebeler, an analytical chemist and oenologist at the University of California, Davis. "Flavour is a multisensory experience, including aroma, taste, colour, mouth-feel and even auditory cues. CO2 can affect many of these senses."

Cold comfort

The slower a glass of bubbly releases its CO2, the longer it remains vivacious. Yet it turns out that most CO2 is lost not through bubbles bursting but by simple diffusion across the liquid surface. That is why flutes are used in the first place: the narrow neck reduces the surface area from which gas can escape.

The correct way to pour champagneTilting the glass helps to keep more carbon dioxide in solution.Gérard Liger-Belair / American Chemical Society

Pouring has a big influence on the gas content, both because the 'tongue' of liquid falling from the bottle to the glass exposes a large surface area and because turbulence and entrapment of air bubbles as the liquid hits the glass can speed up diffusion of CO2 out of solution.

The beer-pouring action should cut CO2 loss on both counts: the column of flowing liquid is less exposed to air, and the gentler impact reduces turbulence. Liger-Belair and his colleagues have confirmed this, using a standard method for measuring dissolved CO2 concentrations (based on the activity of the enzyme carbonic anhydrase, which reacts with carbonic acid), and using a method called thermography, which provides snapshots of CO2 levels in air based on how strongly it absorbs infrared radiation.

The researchers also show that both to maximize CO2 retention and to obtain the full benefits of the beer-pouring technique, the champagne must be chilled. When served at close to room temperature (18 °C), champagne served by the beer technique loses about two and a half times more CO2 than when chilled — almost as much as it loses when served in the traditional way at room temperature.

Liger-Belair and his team show that increased gas loss from warm champagne is mostly a result of two factors. First, the colder liquid is more viscous, and so gas-leaking turbulence is dampened more quickly. Second, CO2 molecules diffuse slowler in cold water, and so are unable to reach the surface as quickly.

Will champagne lovers be persuaded to alter their ways? "Champagne is a universe full of traditions," says Liger–Belair, who has worked as a consultant for the research department of Moët & Chandon. "But maybe champagne and science can mix to offer a better way of tasting," he says. He has at least one convert already. "Based on this paper, I probably will pour using the beer-like method now," says Ebeler. 

  • References

    1. Liger–Belair, G. et al. J. Agric. Food Chem. advance online publication doi:10.1021/jf101239w (2010).

Absolutely vital advice!

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio

Pregabalin and Transcutaneous Electrical Nerve Stimulation for Postherpetic Neuralgia Treatment

Check out this website I found at pdfs.journals.lww.com

Perhaps this synergy will help other conditions that involve neuropathic pain, e.g. back pain that has a NP component. Understanding the TENS parameters will allow us to determine potential mechanisms and hence other ways of facilitating these processes and pathways.

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio

Thursday, 22 July 2010

Serotonin cell discoveries mean rethink of depression

IF YOU thought depression was caused by low serotonin levels, think again. It looks as if the brain chemistry of a depressed person is much more complex, with mounting evidence suggesting that too much serotonin in some brain regions is to blame.

If correct, it might explain some of the negative side-effects associated with selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), antidepressants like Prozac which increase the amount of the neurotransmitter serotonin in some parts of the brain.

The traditional view of depression was largely based on the observation that SSRIs boost mood- although why they do so is unknown. "Because antidepressants increase serotonin in some parts of the brain, people assumed that depression must be the result of low serotonin levels," says Christopher Lowry of the University of Boulder in Colorado. But the discovery of multiple types of serotonin-releasing neurons in the brain, along with high levels of serotonin recorded in people with depression, is prompting a rethink.

"What's more likely is that there are subgroups of serotonin neurons that are overactive in depressed patients, rather than underactive as we have all been assuming," says Lowry.

It's likely there are groups of serotonin neurons that are overactive, not underactive as assumed

One of the first clues that something might be amiss with the traditional theory came three years ago, when Murray Esler at the Baker Heart Research Institute in Melbourne, Australia, and colleagues found that the level of serotonin in the brains of people with panic disorder was four times higher than in healthy volunteers (Stress, DOI: 10.1080/10253890701300904), and in depressed people who were not receiving treatment it was two times higher than in volunteers (Archives of General Psychiatry, vol 65, p 38). They also showed that long-term use of SSRIs in people with depression and panic disorder seemed to decrease serotonin levels through an as yet unidentified mechanism.

Now, in studies of rats and mice, Lowry has found that there are multiple types of serotonin neurons that can be independently regulated. He presented his results at the Forum of European Neuroscience in Amsterdam, the Netherlands, last week.

This fits well with findings from other groups that there are two types of serotonin receptor in the amygdala, a brain region linked to emotion and anxiety: 5-HT2A receptors that inhibit anxiety, and 5-HT2C receptors that promote it. The roles of the receptors were identified by injecting drugs that either stimulated or inhibited each receptor and observing the animals' behavioural response.

Together, the findings might mean that while high levels of serotonin in some brain regions like the prefrontal cortex can lead to improved mood, high serotonin in other regions could have negative effects.

Evidence for this idea comes from Gina Forster at the University of South Dakota in Vermillion and colleagues, who injected a stress-related molecule into the brains of rats and found that it triggered two phases of serotonin release. An initial wave of serotonin appeared to increase fear-like behaviour in the rats, while a second wave decreased this behaviour, possibly because it activated a brain region called the medial prefrontal cortex, which is associated with calming of fears (Neuroscience, vol 141, p 1047).

The new findings have implications for how SSRI drugs work. In the long-term, SSRIs do tend to have a calming effect, although more research is needed to understand how they do this.

However, in the short-term some people taking SSRIs report feeling increased anxiety. This is "almost certainly due to the activation of one of these groups of serotonin neurons", says Lowry. The suicidal thoughts some people taking SSRIs claim to experience may also be linked to boosting serotonin, as suicide is thought to be associated with increased impulsivity. "It may be that certain types of SSRI are affecting these impulsivity circuits in the brain," says Lowry.

Learning more about these different groups of serotonin neurons could lead to better treatments for depression and anxiety disorders. "It might be possible to design very specific drugs that can turn on or off specific groups of neurons that are deregulated in anxiety or depression," says Lowry.

Issue 2770 of New Scientist magazine

  • New Scientist
  • Not just a website!
  • Subscribe to New Scientist and get:
  • New Scientist magazine delivered to your door
  • Unlimited online access to articles from over 500 back issues
  • Subscribe Now and Save

If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.

Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.

Oh Pharmaceuticals. .

Thu Jul 22 18:48:44 BST 2010 by nigel

lol we've (kind of) known about this since 1997: http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/119148194/abstract

-the intro's gives a good overview of how wavy the whole low-seretonin theory was 13 YEARS AGO

..but someone's gotta sell their SSRIs haha

All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.

If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio

Structural brain alterations in patients with irritable bowel syndrome discovered

Brain training needed for IBS?

Posted via email from Specialist Pain Physio